The University of Southampton
University of Southampton Institutional Repository

Susceptibility to COVID-19 Scams: The Roles of Age, Individual Difference Measures, and Scam-Related Perceptions

Susceptibility to COVID-19 Scams: The Roles of Age, Individual Difference Measures, and Scam-Related Perceptions
Susceptibility to COVID-19 Scams: The Roles of Age, Individual Difference Measures, and Scam-Related Perceptions
As the COVID-19 pandemic was unfolding, a surge in scams was registered across the globe. While COVID-19 poses higher health risks for older adults, it is unknown whether older adults are also facing higher financial risks as a result of COVID-19 scams. Here, we examined age differences in vulnerability to COVID-19 scams and individual difference measures (such as impulsivity, ad skepticism, and past experiences with fraud) that might help explain them. A lifespan sample (M = 48.03, SD = 18.56) of sixty-eight younger (18–40 years, M = 25.67, SD = 5.93), 79 middle-aged (41–64 years, M = 49.86, SD = 7.20), and 63 older adults (65–84 years, M = 69.87, SD = 4.50) recruited through Prolific completed questions and questionnaires online. In a within-subjects design, each participant responded to five COVID-19 solicitations, psychological measures, and demographic questions. Age group comparisons revealed
that older adults were marginally less likely to perceive COVID-19 solicitations as genuine than middle-aged adults were. In addition, older adults perceived significantly fewer benefits than both younger and middle-aged adults did and perceived marginally higher risks than younger adults did. Hence, older adults did not exhibit greater vulnerability to COVID-19 scams. Regardless of age, intentions to respond to COVID-19 solicitations were positively predicted by higher levels of educational attainment, being married, past fraud victimization, and higher levels of positive urgency. As expected, stronger genuineness and benefit perceptions positively predicted action intentions, whereas stronger risk perceptions negatively predicted action intentions As such, COVID-19 scam susceptibility appears to be the result of a impulse control issue that is not easily
inhibited, not even by past experiences of scam victimization.
1664-1078
Nolte, Julia
fc15b4bb-c174-487d-b752-4280c51f408e
Hanoch, Yaniv
3cf08e80-8bda-4d3b-af1c-46c858aa9f39
Wood, Stacey
f73c11f1-c315-4812-9eb9-5d4cb5b395ad
Hengerer, David
26ab7898-603e-4caa-a8f4-49938dcedaef
Nolte, Julia
fc15b4bb-c174-487d-b752-4280c51f408e
Hanoch, Yaniv
3cf08e80-8bda-4d3b-af1c-46c858aa9f39
Wood, Stacey
f73c11f1-c315-4812-9eb9-5d4cb5b395ad
Hengerer, David
26ab7898-603e-4caa-a8f4-49938dcedaef

Nolte, Julia, Hanoch, Yaniv, Wood, Stacey and Hengerer, David (2021) Susceptibility to COVID-19 Scams: The Roles of Age, Individual Difference Measures, and Scam-Related Perceptions. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, [789883]. (doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.789883).

Record type: Article

Abstract

As the COVID-19 pandemic was unfolding, a surge in scams was registered across the globe. While COVID-19 poses higher health risks for older adults, it is unknown whether older adults are also facing higher financial risks as a result of COVID-19 scams. Here, we examined age differences in vulnerability to COVID-19 scams and individual difference measures (such as impulsivity, ad skepticism, and past experiences with fraud) that might help explain them. A lifespan sample (M = 48.03, SD = 18.56) of sixty-eight younger (18–40 years, M = 25.67, SD = 5.93), 79 middle-aged (41–64 years, M = 49.86, SD = 7.20), and 63 older adults (65–84 years, M = 69.87, SD = 4.50) recruited through Prolific completed questions and questionnaires online. In a within-subjects design, each participant responded to five COVID-19 solicitations, psychological measures, and demographic questions. Age group comparisons revealed
that older adults were marginally less likely to perceive COVID-19 solicitations as genuine than middle-aged adults were. In addition, older adults perceived significantly fewer benefits than both younger and middle-aged adults did and perceived marginally higher risks than younger adults did. Hence, older adults did not exhibit greater vulnerability to COVID-19 scams. Regardless of age, intentions to respond to COVID-19 solicitations were positively predicted by higher levels of educational attainment, being married, past fraud victimization, and higher levels of positive urgency. As expected, stronger genuineness and benefit perceptions positively predicted action intentions, whereas stronger risk perceptions negatively predicted action intentions As such, COVID-19 scam susceptibility appears to be the result of a impulse control issue that is not easily
inhibited, not even by past experiences of scam victimization.

This record has no associated files available for download.

More information

Accepted/In Press date: 23 November 2021
e-pub ahead of print date: 15 December 2021
Published date: 15 December 2021
Additional Information: Funding Information: This research was self-funded. Publisher Copyright: Copyright © 2021 Nolte, Hanoch, Wood and Hengerer.

Identifiers

Local EPrints ID: 453075
URI: http://eprints.soton.ac.uk/id/eprint/453075
ISSN: 1664-1078
PURE UUID: 0d66cdc2-2854-490e-ae36-0c76da00098e
ORCID for Yaniv Hanoch: ORCID iD orcid.org/0000-0001-9453-4588

Catalogue record

Date deposited: 07 Jan 2022 17:51
Last modified: 10 Jul 2024 20:31

Export record

Altmetrics

Contributors

Author: Julia Nolte
Author: Yaniv Hanoch ORCID iD
Author: Stacey Wood
Author: David Hengerer

Download statistics

Downloads from ePrints over the past year. Other digital versions may also be available to download e.g. from the publisher's website.

View more statistics

Atom RSS 1.0 RSS 2.0

Contact ePrints Soton: eprints@soton.ac.uk

ePrints Soton supports OAI 2.0 with a base URL of http://eprints.soton.ac.uk/cgi/oai2

This repository has been built using EPrints software, developed at the University of Southampton, but available to everyone to use.

We use cookies to ensure that we give you the best experience on our website. If you continue without changing your settings, we will assume that you are happy to receive cookies on the University of Southampton website.

×