The University of Southampton
University of Southampton Institutional Repository

Computerized cognitive training in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials with blinded and objective outcomes

Computerized cognitive training in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials with blinded and objective outcomes
Computerized cognitive training in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials with blinded and objective outcomes
This meta-analysis investigated the effects of computerized cognitive training (CCT) on clinical, neuropsychological and academic outcomes in individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The authors searched PubMed, Ovid, and Web of Science until 19th January 2022 for parallel-arm randomized controlled trials (RCTs) using CCT in individuals with ADHD. Random-effects meta-analyses pooled standardized mean differences (SMD) between CCT and comparator arms. RCT quality was assessed with the Cochrane Risk of Bias 2.0 tool (PROSPERO: CRD42021229279). Thirty-six RCTs were meta-analysed, 17 of which evaluated working memory training (WMT). Analysis of outcomes measured immediately post-treatment and judged to be “probably blinded” (PBLIND; trial n = 14) showed no effect on ADHD total (SMD = 0.12, 95%CI[−0.01 to −0.25]) or hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms (SMD = 0.12, 95%[−0.03 to−0.28]). These findings remained when analyses were restricted to trials (n: 5–13) with children/adolescents, low medication exposure, semi-active controls, or WMT or multiple process training. There was a small improvement in inattention symptoms (SMD = 0.17, 95%CI[0.02–0.31]), which remained when trials were restricted to semi-active controls (SMD = 0.20, 95%CI[0.04–0.37]), and doubled in size when assessed in the intervention delivery setting (n = 5, SMD = 0.40, 95%CI[0.09–0.71]), suggesting a setting-specific effect. CCT improved WM (verbal: n = 15, SMD = 0.38, 95%CI[0.24–0.53]; visual-spatial: n = 9, SMD = 0.49, 95%CI[0.31–0.67]), but not other neuropsychological (e.g., attention, inhibition) or academic outcomes (e.g., reading, arithmetic; analysed n: 5–15). Longer-term improvement (at ~6-months) in verbal WM, reading comprehension, and ratings of executive functions were observed but relevant trials were limited in number (n: 5–7). There was no evidence that multi-process training was superior to working memory training. In sum, CCT led to shorter-term improvements in WM, with some evidence that verbal WM effects persisted in the longer-term. Clinical effects were limited to small, setting specific, short-term effects on inattention symptoms.
1359-4184
1402–1414
Westwood, Samuel J.
4683dcf1-f7bb-4d3a-beea-6e554a3d538b
Parlatini, Valeria
6cdfb200-40ce-43ce-84da-dcb6eba0f67a
Rubia, Katya
5f6c0771-6e32-4924-b3f1-e48ace6de377
Cortese, Samuele
53d4bf2c-4e0e-4c77-9385-218350560fdb
Sonuga-Barke, Edmund J.S.
f828d850-99b4-4724-9870-183a038cd630
Banaschewski, T.
ca2651e5-143a-4171-b6d0-3e3853b684d8
Baeyens, D.
a1b81e34-c9b0-45e5-bbcf-ca3ad294201a
Bölte, S.
1cc87f1a-18f1-4c73-aebc-8793e6856f85
Brandeis, D.
b3f7c424-d307-476e-aac8-edff6ca08921
Buitelaar, J.
fa22aba5-1307-4627-9ae8-e564013a5af8
Carucci, S.
2742f5af-0769-49bb-a830-8c49a05c4f5d
Coghill, D.
a4b982d1-4788-41aa-90c5-b4a7f7dffad7
Daley, D.
5435e844-1cad-40a7-9d75-d8bed0cc72eb
Döpfner, M.
4b8e28d1-48ee-434e-b6cd-ede9dd9f1caa
Galera, C.
af967cde-ac35-4a57-a361-255070386576
Hollis, C.
dc8c2836-32b3-4f5a-a53a-aeeef1ae0334
Holtmann, M.
44c88726-6e97-4b7e-b0e5-b2e7780e31c2
Purper-Ouakil, D.
e90fed7c-b091-4b3f-a888-ac448f156946
Nagy, P.
0dac0a38-2479-4299-950c-09c79b3d93e1
Santosh, P.
83759ca4-e254-4733-be8d-df022df6ce89
Simonoff, E.
23762462-75a9-41bd-9c1e-2cb8e3cf8852
Soutullo, C.A.
87e1ae8f-1a11-4df5-aa79-34fce7ffc0ad
Stringaris, A.
b813012e-7b67-432b-9799-2187bf675c30
Thapar, A.
792558b3-01ec-4ccf-83c6-9f408aa3b601
Oord, S. van der
c6f89aff-e14e-419d-9e32-98f91656956f
Hoofdakker, B.J. van den
bee4f1e3-e9b5-405f-b413-9bcd99fc946b
Zuddas, A.
487c598f-2f61-4f20-bef7-dcdbbdfb4633
European ADHD Guidelines Group (EAGG)
Westwood, Samuel J.
4683dcf1-f7bb-4d3a-beea-6e554a3d538b
Parlatini, Valeria
6cdfb200-40ce-43ce-84da-dcb6eba0f67a
Rubia, Katya
5f6c0771-6e32-4924-b3f1-e48ace6de377
Cortese, Samuele
53d4bf2c-4e0e-4c77-9385-218350560fdb
Sonuga-Barke, Edmund J.S.
f828d850-99b4-4724-9870-183a038cd630
Banaschewski, T.
ca2651e5-143a-4171-b6d0-3e3853b684d8
Baeyens, D.
a1b81e34-c9b0-45e5-bbcf-ca3ad294201a
Bölte, S.
1cc87f1a-18f1-4c73-aebc-8793e6856f85
Brandeis, D.
b3f7c424-d307-476e-aac8-edff6ca08921
Buitelaar, J.
fa22aba5-1307-4627-9ae8-e564013a5af8
Carucci, S.
2742f5af-0769-49bb-a830-8c49a05c4f5d
Coghill, D.
a4b982d1-4788-41aa-90c5-b4a7f7dffad7
Daley, D.
5435e844-1cad-40a7-9d75-d8bed0cc72eb
Döpfner, M.
4b8e28d1-48ee-434e-b6cd-ede9dd9f1caa
Galera, C.
af967cde-ac35-4a57-a361-255070386576
Hollis, C.
dc8c2836-32b3-4f5a-a53a-aeeef1ae0334
Holtmann, M.
44c88726-6e97-4b7e-b0e5-b2e7780e31c2
Purper-Ouakil, D.
e90fed7c-b091-4b3f-a888-ac448f156946
Nagy, P.
0dac0a38-2479-4299-950c-09c79b3d93e1
Santosh, P.
83759ca4-e254-4733-be8d-df022df6ce89
Simonoff, E.
23762462-75a9-41bd-9c1e-2cb8e3cf8852
Soutullo, C.A.
87e1ae8f-1a11-4df5-aa79-34fce7ffc0ad
Stringaris, A.
b813012e-7b67-432b-9799-2187bf675c30
Thapar, A.
792558b3-01ec-4ccf-83c6-9f408aa3b601
Oord, S. van der
c6f89aff-e14e-419d-9e32-98f91656956f
Hoofdakker, B.J. van den
bee4f1e3-e9b5-405f-b413-9bcd99fc946b
Zuddas, A.
487c598f-2f61-4f20-bef7-dcdbbdfb4633

Westwood, Samuel J., Parlatini, Valeria, Rubia, Katya, Cortese, Samuele and Sonuga-Barke, Edmund J.S. , European ADHD Guidelines Group (EAGG) (2023) Computerized cognitive training in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials with blinded and objective outcomes. Molecular Psychiatry, 28, 1402–1414. (doi:10.1038/s41380-023-02000-7).

Record type: Article

Abstract

This meta-analysis investigated the effects of computerized cognitive training (CCT) on clinical, neuropsychological and academic outcomes in individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). The authors searched PubMed, Ovid, and Web of Science until 19th January 2022 for parallel-arm randomized controlled trials (RCTs) using CCT in individuals with ADHD. Random-effects meta-analyses pooled standardized mean differences (SMD) between CCT and comparator arms. RCT quality was assessed with the Cochrane Risk of Bias 2.0 tool (PROSPERO: CRD42021229279). Thirty-six RCTs were meta-analysed, 17 of which evaluated working memory training (WMT). Analysis of outcomes measured immediately post-treatment and judged to be “probably blinded” (PBLIND; trial n = 14) showed no effect on ADHD total (SMD = 0.12, 95%CI[−0.01 to −0.25]) or hyperactivity/impulsivity symptoms (SMD = 0.12, 95%[−0.03 to−0.28]). These findings remained when analyses were restricted to trials (n: 5–13) with children/adolescents, low medication exposure, semi-active controls, or WMT or multiple process training. There was a small improvement in inattention symptoms (SMD = 0.17, 95%CI[0.02–0.31]), which remained when trials were restricted to semi-active controls (SMD = 0.20, 95%CI[0.04–0.37]), and doubled in size when assessed in the intervention delivery setting (n = 5, SMD = 0.40, 95%CI[0.09–0.71]), suggesting a setting-specific effect. CCT improved WM (verbal: n = 15, SMD = 0.38, 95%CI[0.24–0.53]; visual-spatial: n = 9, SMD = 0.49, 95%CI[0.31–0.67]), but not other neuropsychological (e.g., attention, inhibition) or academic outcomes (e.g., reading, arithmetic; analysed n: 5–15). Longer-term improvement (at ~6-months) in verbal WM, reading comprehension, and ratings of executive functions were observed but relevant trials were limited in number (n: 5–7). There was no evidence that multi-process training was superior to working memory training. In sum, CCT led to shorter-term improvements in WM, with some evidence that verbal WM effects persisted in the longer-term. Clinical effects were limited to small, setting specific, short-term effects on inattention symptoms.

Text
s41380-023-02000-7 - Version of Record
Available under License Creative Commons Attribution.
Download (2MB)

More information

Accepted/In Press date: 10 February 2023
Published date: 29 March 2023

Identifiers

Local EPrints ID: 489024
URI: http://eprints.soton.ac.uk/id/eprint/489024
ISSN: 1359-4184
PURE UUID: 159ee5d3-f13b-47e9-a832-68d760fae1a6
ORCID for Valeria Parlatini: ORCID iD orcid.org/0000-0002-4754-2494
ORCID for Samuele Cortese: ORCID iD orcid.org/0000-0001-5877-8075

Catalogue record

Date deposited: 11 Apr 2024 16:38
Last modified: 10 Sep 2024 02:09

Export record

Altmetrics

Contributors

Author: Samuel J. Westwood
Author: Valeria Parlatini ORCID iD
Author: Katya Rubia
Author: Samuele Cortese ORCID iD
Author: Edmund J.S. Sonuga-Barke
Author: T. Banaschewski
Author: D. Baeyens
Author: S. Bölte
Author: D. Brandeis
Author: J. Buitelaar
Author: S. Carucci
Author: D. Coghill
Author: D. Daley
Author: M. Döpfner
Author: C. Galera
Author: C. Hollis
Author: M. Holtmann
Author: D. Purper-Ouakil
Author: P. Nagy
Author: P. Santosh
Author: E. Simonoff
Author: C.A. Soutullo
Author: A. Stringaris
Author: A. Thapar
Author: S. van der Oord
Author: B.J. van den Hoofdakker
Author: A. Zuddas
Corporate Author: European ADHD Guidelines Group (EAGG)

Download statistics

Downloads from ePrints over the past year. Other digital versions may also be available to download e.g. from the publisher's website.

View more statistics

Atom RSS 1.0 RSS 2.0

Contact ePrints Soton: eprints@soton.ac.uk

ePrints Soton supports OAI 2.0 with a base URL of http://eprints.soton.ac.uk/cgi/oai2

This repository has been built using EPrints software, developed at the University of Southampton, but available to everyone to use.

We use cookies to ensure that we give you the best experience on our website. If you continue without changing your settings, we will assume that you are happy to receive cookies on the University of Southampton website.

×